Miles David Storfer (2001) The Parallel Increase in Brain Size, Intelligence, and Myopia. Psycoloquy: 12(013) Brain Intelligence (5)

Volume: 12 (next, prev) Issue: 013 (next, prev) Article: 5 (next prev first) Alternate versions: ASCII Summary
Topic:
Article:
PSYCOLOQUY (ISSN 1055-0143) is sponsored by the American Psychological Association (APA).
Psycoloquy 12(013): The Parallel Increase in Brain Size, Intelligence, and Myopia

THE PARALLEL INCREASE IN BRAIN SIZE, INTELLIGENCE, AND MYOPIA
Reply to Uner Tan on Brain-Intelligence

Miles David Storfer
The Foundation for Brain Research
46 Brittany A Drive
Delray Beach, FL 33446
USA

brainfoundation@aol.com

Abstract

Tan judges my target article as excellent, but expresses the view that in analyzing the long-term increases in brain size, myopia and intelligence, insufficient attention was paid to sex differences, handedness and sex hormones. I respond to some propositions advanced by Tan, and discuss these areas, including alternative interpretations for some of Tan's findings.

Keywords

allergy, brain size, development, evolution, gene expression regulation, genomic imprinting, gifted, intelligence, myopia, neocortex.
1. Tan states that gender differences should have been considered in more detail, since 'the increase in IQ could all be due to females.' I did consider gender, but rejected Tan's hypothesis of a substantially greater female change in recent decades on three grounds: (1) no mention of gender-disparate secular trends appeared in Lynn's (1994) analysis of gender differences in IQ in the Western and far-Eastern worlds; (2) the head circumference norms for male and female children in the US have risen at equivalent rates (a full standard deviation) between the 1920's and 1970s (Ounstead, Moar & Scott 1985; Whitehead & Paul 1988); and (3) in England, the long-term increase in men's and women's brain weight has been at the same rate since the turn of the century (Miller & Corsellis 1977).

2. Conditions may differ in Tan's Turkish populations, where educational conditions for females may have recently undergone great improvement. Indeed, Miller & Corsellis (1977) found that, among 7,400 people age 20-50 autopsied in London, the trend toward increasing brain weight in the female began roughly thirty years later (1900) than in the male.

3. Also, the subjects of all of Tan's numerous studies were students at a Turkish medical faculty (and, in his MRI study, students at a high school for sports were also included). Thus, their brain morphologies are apt to differ from the general population (both on account of their very high IQs, and perhaps, their special talents). Although this would limit the generalizability of Tan's findings, his data would be very useful in a meta-analysis of the relationship between gifts of intellect/physicality and brain organization.

4. With respect to handedness, Tan asserts that if my theory is correct, rather than remaining nearly constant since early history, the proportion of left-handers should have risen in parallel with intelligence, since many studies report a relation between IQ and handedness. In response: first, I do not share Tan's belief that an intergenerational rise in the prevalence of left-handers has not occurred, since several parent-offspring studies led me to conclude that it had (e.g., Flemington, Dalton & Standage 1977; Porac, Coren & Duncan 1980; L. Tan 1983). In (Leslie) Tan's paper, 11.8% of the (917 Australian) offspring were left handed compared with 5.9% of their parents, and it was observed that large intergenerational differences were seen on tasks where there was unlikely to have been substantial social pressure for right-handedness in the earlier generation.

5. Second, although it's very well established that disproportionately more left-handers are found among people who've attained eminence in a variety of creative endeavours, this left-handed advantage diminishes, then reverses as one extends to lower IQs. I previously reported on the handedness of 2,720 adult members of high-IQ societies (using a modification of the Oldfield Handedness Test; see Storfer 1995), and classified 11.5% of the men and 10.5% of the women as left handed, and a further 12.5% of the men and 12.1% of the women as near- ambidextrous. These totals are larger than usually observed (Geschwind & Behan 1984), especially for females.

6. At least one large study (Hardyck, Petrinovich & Goldman 1976) found no difference in the average IQs of (5,600) left-and-right-handed schoolchildren, although the left-handers displayed considerably more IQ variability. I think this excess variability arises from there being 3 distinct brain organizations that can produce sinstrality: (1) hemispheric-reversal cases (15% or so of lefties) who, because of uterine problems, have IQs that average about a full standard deviation below the norm; (2) lefties with an otherwise 'normal' brain configuration (most lefties); and (3) people who are 'bilateralized' for speech (i.e., who have speech centers in both hemispheres), a phenomenon which, based on findings from open-brain operations (Ojemann 1983) or the observed effects of major stroke (c.f. Bradshaw & Nettleton 1983), occurs in about 15%-20% of left-handed people and 1%-2% of right-handers: implying that about half the bilateralized population is left handed (see Storfer, 1990; ch. 17). If the trend toward increasing IQ and brain size is making bilaterality more common, this would raise the percentage of left-handers. However, better uterine and neonatal care may be reducing hemispheric-reversal as a source of left handedness (and thereby raising IQ while reducing sinstrality).

7. Tan has intensively studied the relationship between testosterone levels and performance on spatial-reasoning problems, and reports that, among male (but not female) 'pre-med' students, a positive correlation exists. Thus, he asserts that, if intelligence and brain size were really increasing rapidly, we should have seen a large increase in the proportion of men and women having high testosterone levels (which we have not) and a shift to greater right-hemisphere activity. My analysis of data from Tan's 1990(c) study yields a different conclusion: 8 of the 19 right-handed male students in the moderate serum-testosterone- level range (550-950 ng/dl) had IQs above 130, compared with only 1 of the 6 students with higher levels, and none of the 6 with lower levels. Tan's data in a later paper (Tan & Tan 1998) display a steep progressive IQ fall among women students at levels above 500 ng/dl and a modest IQ decline among men at levels above 1,000 ng/dl.

8. I accept the idea that the higher levels of testosterone in the male brain are causally related the slight advantage men display on tasks of (three-dimensional) spatial reasoning, but I don't accept that this generalizes, either to the verbal or other non-spatial aspects of IQ, or their underlying biology. I base this on the following kinds of evidence: (1) other than in the 3-D spatial domain, males and females perform equally well on IQ-tests (Lynn 1994); (2) more difficult Ravens Matrices test problems evoke much additional left-brain, and little additional right-brain activity (Haier et al. 1983); (3) the primate studies cited in the Precis (paragraph 25) show a far greater intergenerational growth in the left hemisphere's speech strategy area than in the right; and (4) the development of the human intellect is thought to have been accompanied by a greater increase in neuronal activity in the left hemisphere than in the right (left hemisphere activation being associated with quiet concentration, conceptual- analytical processing and self-reflective thought, while the right is conceived as our externally-focused vigilant, perceptual- holistic processor). This is particularly true in language processing areas, where the two hemispheres differ greatly in neuronal architecture -- and where the left-hemisphere neurons fire far more often, both because they have many more dendrites, and because proportionately more dendrites receive excitatory transmissions (Scheibel et al. 1985).

REFERENCES

Bradshaw, J. L. & Nettleton, N. C. (1983) Human Cerebral Asymmetry. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall

Flemington, J. J., Dalton, R. & Standage, K. F. (1977) Age as a factor in the handedness of adults. Neuropsychologia 15: 471-473.

Geschwind, N. & Behan, P. (1984) Left-handedness: association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 79: 5097-5100.

Hardyck, C., Petrinovich, L. F. & Goldman, R. F. (1976) Left handedness and cognitive deficits. Cortex 12: 266-279.

Haier, R. J., et al. (1983) Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography. Intelligence 12: 199-217.

Lynn, R. (1994) Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: a paradox resolved. Personality & Individual Differences 17: 257-271.

Miller, A. K. H. & Corsellis, J. A. N. (1977) Evidence for a secular increase in human brain weight during the past century. Annals of Human Biology 4: 253-257.

Ojemann, G. (1983) Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6: 189-230.

Ounstead M., Moar, V. & Scott, A. (1985) Head circumference charts updated. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 60, 936-939.

Porac, C., Coren, S. & Duncan, P. (1980) Life-span age trends in laterality. Journal of Gerontology 35: 715-721.

Scheibel, A. B. et al. (1985) Differentiating characteristics of the human speech cortex. In D. F. Benson & E. Zaidel (Eds.), The Dual Brain. New York: Guilford Press.

Storfer, M. D. (1990) Intelligence and Giftedness: The Contributions of Heredity and Early Environment. San Francisco & Oxford, Jossey-Bass.

Storfer, M. D. (1995) Problems in left-right discrimination in a high-IQ population. Perceptual and Motor Skills 81: 491-497.

Storfer, M. (2000) Precis of "Brain size, intelligence and myopia" PSYCOLOQUY 11(083) ftp://ftp.princeton.edu/pub/harnad/Psycoloquy/2000.volume.11/ psyc.00.11.083.brain-intelligence.1.storfer http://www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?11.083

Tan, L. (1983) Handedness in two generations. Perceptual and Motor Skills 56: 867-874.

Tan, U., & Tan, M. (1998). The curvelinear correlations between the total testosterone levels and fluid intelligence in men and women. International Journal of Neuroscience, 94: 55-61.

Tan, U.(2000) Parallel increase in myopia, brain size, and intelligence following urbanization. PSYCOLOQUY 11(102) ftp://ftp.princeton.edu/pub/harnad/Psycoloquy/2000.volume.11/ psyc.00.11.102.brain-intelligence.2.tan http://www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/newpsy?11.102

Whitehead, R. G. & Paul, A. A. (1988) Comparative infant nutrition in man and animals. Proceedings on the International Symposium on Comparative Nutrition. London: Libby


Volume: 12 (next, prev) Issue: 013 (next, prev) Article: 5 (next prev first) Alternate versions: ASCII Summary
Topic:
Article: